The Basic Needs
7 minutes • 1376 words
Table of contents
The ‘Physiological’ Needs
The physiological drives are usually the starting point for motivation theory.
Two recent findings have been made in this regard:
- The development of the concept of homeostasis
- The finding that appetites (preferential choices among foods) efficiently indicate the actual needs in the body
Homeostasis is the body’s automatic efforts to maintain a constant, normal state of the blood stream.*
Superphysics Note
Cannon (2) has described this process for:
- (1) the water content of the blood
- (2) salt content
- (3) sugar content
- (4) protein content
- (5) fat content
- (6) calcium content
- (7) oxygen content
- (8) constant hydrogen-ion level (acid-base balance)
- (9) constant temperature of the blood.
Obviously, this list can be extended to include other minerals, the hormones, vitamins, etc.
Young in a recent article (21) has summarized the work on appetite in its relation to body needs. If the body lacks some chemical, the individual will tend to develop a specific appetite or partial hunger for that food element.
Thus it is impossible and useless to make a list of fundamental physiological needs. This is because they can be in any number.
We can not identify all physiological needs as homeostatic.
It has not yet been demonstrated whether sexual desire, sleepiness, sheer activity and maternal behavior in animals, are homeostatic.*
Superphysics Note
This list would not include the various sensory pleasures (tastes, smells, tickling, stroking) which are probably physiological and which may become the goals of motivated behavior.
A previous paper pointed out that these physiological needs are unusual rather than typical. This is because:
- they are isolable
- they are localizable somatically.
They are relatively independent of each other, of other motivations and of the organism as a whole,
In many cases, it is possible to demonstrate a localized, underlying somatic base for the drive.
This is true less generally than has been thought (exceptions are fatigue, sleepiness, maternal responses) but it is still true in the classic instances of hunger, sex, and thirst.
Any of the physiological needs and the consummatory behavior involved with them serve as channels for all sorts of other needs as well.
The person who thinks he is hungry may actually be seeking more for comfort, or dependence, than for vitamins or proteins.*
Superphysics Note
Conversely, it is possible to satisfy the hunger need in part by other activities such as drinking water or smoking cigarettes.
In other words, relatively isolable as these physiological needs are, they are not completely so.
These physiological needs are the most pre-potent of all needs.
In the human being who is missing everything in life in an extreme fashion, it is most likely that the major motivation would be the physiological needs rather than any others.
A person who is lacking food, safety, love, and esteem would most probably hunger for food more strongly than for anything else.
If all the needs are unsatisfied, and the organism is then dominated by the physiological needs, all other needs may become simply non-existent or be pushed into the background.
It is then fair to characterize the whole organism by saying simply that it is hungry, for consciousness is almost completely preempted by hunger.
All capacities are put into the service of hunger-satisfaction, and the organization of these capacities is almost entirely determined by the one purpose of satisfying hunger.
The receptors and effectors, the intelligence, memory, habits, all may now be defined simply as hunger-gratifying tools.
Capacities that are not useful for this purpose lie dormant, or are pushed into the background. The urge to write poetry, the desire to acquire an automobile, the interest in American history, the desire for a new pair of shoes are, in the extreme case, forgotten or become of secondary importance.
The man who is extremely and dangerously hungry, no other interests exist but food.
He dreams food, he remembers food, he thinks about food, he emotes only about food, he perceives only food and he wants only food. The more subtle determinants that ordinarily fuse with the physiological drives in organizing even feeding, drinking or sexual behavior, may now be so completely overwhelmed as to allow us to speak at this time (but only at this time) of pure hunger drive and behavior, with the one unqualified aim of relief.
Another peculiar characteristic of the human organism when it is dominated by a certain need is that the whole philosophy of the future tends also to change. For our chronically and extremely hungry man, Utopia can be defined very simply as a place where there is plenty of food.
He tends to think that, if only he is guaranteed food for the rest of his life, he will be perfectly happy and will never want anything more. Life itself tends to be defined in terms of eating. Anything else will be defined as unimportant. Freedom, love, community feeling, respect, philosophy, may all be waved aside as fripperies which are useless since they fail to fill the stomach. Such a man may fairly be said to live by bread alone.
Their generality can be denied.
Emergency conditions are, almost by definition, rare in the normally functioning peaceful society. This truism can be forgotten due to 2 reasons:
- Rats have few motivations other than physiological ones.
Since so much of the research on motivation has been made with rats, it is easy to carry the rat-picture over to the human being.
- Culture itself is an adaptive tool.
One of its main functions is to make the physiological emergencies come less and less often.
In most of the known societies, chronic extreme hunger of the emergency type is rare, rather than common.
In any case, this is still true in the United States. The average American citizen is experiencing appetite rather than hunger when he says “I am hungry.”
He is apt to experience sheer life-and-death hunger only by accident and then only a few times through his entire life.
Obviously, a good way to obscure the ‘higher’ motivations, and to get a lopsided view of human capacities and human nature, is to make the organism extremely and chronically hungry or thirsty.
Anyone who attempts to make an emergency picture into a typical one, and who will measure all of man’s goals and desires by his behavior during extreme physiological deprivation is certainly being blind to many things. It is quite true that man lives by bread alone – when there is no bread. But what happens to man’s desires when there is plenty of bread and when his belly is chronically filled?
At once other (and ‘higher’) needs emerge and these, rather than physiological hungers, dominate the organism. And when these in turn are satisfied, again new (and still ‘higher’) needs emerge and so on. This is what we mean by saying that the basic human needs are organized into a hierarchy of relative prepotency.
One main implication of this phrasing is that gratification becomes as important a concept as deprivation in motivation theory, for it releases the organism from the domination of a relatively more physiological need, permitting thereby the emergence of other more social goals.
The physiological needs, along with their partial goals, when chronically gratified cease to exist as active determinants or organizers of behavior.
They now exist only in a potential fashion in the sense that they may emerge again to dominate the organism if they are thwarted.
But a want that is satisfied is no longer a want. The organism is dominated and its behavior organized only by unsatisfied needs. If hunger is satisfied, it becomes unimportant in the current dynamics of the individual.
This statement is somewhat qualified by a hypothesis to be discussed more fully later, namely that it is precisely those individuals in whom a certain need has always been satisfied who are best equipped to tolerate deprivation of that need in the future, and that furthermore, those who have been de-[p. 376]prived in the past will react differently to current satisfactions than the one who has never been deprived.